WWW.BOOK.DISLIB.INFO
FREE ELECTRONIC LIBRARY - Books, dissertations, abstract
 
<< HOME
CONTACTS



Pages:   || 2 |

«Introduction On emotion tasks, preschoolers frequently select the anger face as disgusted and the disgust face as angry. • e.g, Preschoolers (3 to ...»

-- [ Page 1 ] --

Children’s Understanding of Anger and Disgust: ‘Confusions’ or Overlapping Categories?

Sherri C. Widen & James A. Russell

Boston College

Presented at the International Society for Research on Emotions Conference, Atlanta, 2006

Abstract

Compared to children who don’t use ‘disgusted’ on a labeling task, do those who do better

understand of the difference between anger and disgust? Children (N=144; 3 to 7 years) labeled

facial expressions, and did two categorization tasks. Children who used ‘disgusted’ on the labeling task were better able to exclude the disgust faces from the anger categories on both the categorization tasks. But using disgusted had little effect on children’s disgust category.

Children’s anger and disgust categories are overlapping. This overlap is due to these emotions’ similarity on the dimensions of pleasure and arousal rather than some random error pattern.

Introduction On emotion tasks, preschoolers frequently select the anger face as disgusted and the disgust face as angry.

• e.g, Preschoolers (3 to 5 years) were asked to choose three facial expressions from an array of nine different facial expressions (Bullock & Russell, 1984, 1985).

• For angry, one third of the children chose the disgust face first.

• Children were read brief emotion stories which included the emotion label, and were asked to choose the protagonist’s facial expression from an array of six facial expressions (Gosselin & Laroque, 2000).

• Five-year-olds frequently chose the disgust face for angry and the angry face for disgusted.

• On a free labeling task, when preschoolers labeled a disgust facial expression, they labeled it angry far more frequently than they label it disgusted (Widen & Russell, 2003, 2006).

The question is, how to interpret this response pattern?

• On one perspective, this response pattern has been coined ‘confusions’ between hypothesized universally recognized facial expressions of emotion.

• The assumption of the ‘confusions’ interpretation is that participants’ underlying emotion categories are discrete (e.g., Ekman, 1994; Izard, 1994).

• But confusions between anger and disgust facial expressions (see Figure 1) are curious if assumptions about their universal nature are accepted.

• Universality carries with it evolutionary implications: Presumably, each facial expression evolved for different reasons and should therefore communicate different information (e.g., Fridlund, 1994).

Widen & Russell (ISRE 2006) 1 Anger is an interpersonal emotion, its function is to regain control of a situation and its action tendency is to attack or threaten (Frijda, 1986); its facial expression communicates aggression and a warning to steer clear (Fridlund, 1994).

Disgust, on the other hand, relates primarily to food: its function is protection and its action tendency is rejection (Frijda, 1986); its facial expression communicates smelling something distasteful or expelling something that tastes bad from one’s mouth.

Thus, in their origins, there is no evidence of overlap in the communicative intent of the anger and disgust expressions.

• In addition, the FACS coding (Ekman & Friesen, 1978) of the anger and disgust facial expressions indicates that they do not share anything but incidental action units (e.g., both may have an open mouth).

We suggest an alternative view to explain this pattern of responses: These responses represent genuine, accurate interpretations of the anger and disgust facial expressions based on the underlying dimensions of pleasure and arousal.

• These two broad dimensions are the basis of adults’ emotion judgments (e.g., Russell, 1980;

2003) and provide the basis of children's earliest conceptual system for emotion (e.g., Russell & Bullock, 1986a, 1986b).

• Similarity on these two dimensions allow emotions to be arrayed in a circular structure in the two-dimensional geometric pleasure-arousal space (Figure 2) (Russell, 1980).

• According to this circumplex model, errors are more likely to fall to those emotional facial expressions that are adjacent to the target emotion, and least likely to fall to those emotions opposite the target.

• Disgust and anger are adjacent in this model, and thus have similar levels of pleasure and arousal, making ‘confusions’ between these expressions predictable.

Widen & Russell (ISRE 2006) 2 The Study This study was designed to investigate the development of children’s use of labels for anger and disgust, and the nature of their underlying concepts of these emotions.

• Because preschoolers rarely use disgusted on free labeling (Widen & Russell, 2003, under review), we extended our sample to include kindergarten and Grade 1 students, but still included preschoolers (3 to 5 years) to better trace the development of children’s understanding of disgust.

• We also included a group of university-aged adults.

Children first participated in a brief conversation about emotions, to make the target labels/concepts more accessible, then labeled three animals.

• All participants labeled two sets of six facial expressions to establish whether they knew/used ‘disgusted’ (or some close synonym).





• And completed two categorization tasks (yes/no, choice from array) which assessed the nature of their anger and disgust categories.

Method Participants. Participants were 144 children, all proficient in English and enrolled in daycares in

the Greater Boston, MA, area. There were 24 boys and 24 girls in each of three age groups:

Young (29 to 48 months; mean = 43.6 months, sd = 3.8) and older preschoolers (54 to 69 Widen & Russell (ISRE 2006) 3 months; mean = 61.8 months, sd = 3.3), and Kindergarten and 1st Graders (63 to 94 months;

mean = 78.4 months, sd = 8.46). A group of 48 university-aged adults were also included; they received course credit in exchange for their participation.

Materials Photographs of animals. The animal pictures were four color photographs, one each of a cat, dog, horse, and cow.

Photographs of facial expressions for free labeling. Two sets of 6 black and white 5” x 7” photographs (each posed by a different woman) of prototypical facial expressions of emotion (happiness, sadness, anger, fear, surprise, disgust) were selected from Ekman and Friesen’s (1976) Pictures of Facial Affect.

Photographs of facial expressions for yes/no task. Three sets of 4 facial expressions (happiness, fear, anger, disgust), each posed by a different woman, were selected from the Montreal Set of Facial Displays of Emotion (Beaupré, Cheung, & Hess, 2000).

Photographs of facial expressions for choice-from-array task. Three sets of 6 facial expressions (surprise, happiness, sadness, disgust, anger, fear), all posed by women, were selected from Ekman and Friesen’s (1976) Pictures of Facial Affect. No facial expression was used twice in this task, but 4 of the faces used in the choice-from-array task were also used in free labeling.

Procedure The experimenter spent the first visit getting to know each child. On a subsequent visit, the experimenter invited an individual child to play a game with her.

Priming The experimenter initiated a conversation in which each of the target emotion labels (happy, sad, angry, scared, surprised, disgusted) was introduced by saying, “Today we are going to play a game about feelings. Feelings are like when you feel happy or sad. Do you ever feel happy?

What about sad? Do you ever feel sad?” And so on, until each of the target emotion labels had been mentioned.

Labeling facial expressions The order of presentation for the two sets of facial expression was counterbalanced: Half the children saw Rhonda first; half Alice. The experimenter introduced the faces then showed the child the six facial expressions, one at a time in a random order. After each picture, the experimenter asked, “How do you think Ronda (Alice) feels in this picture?” Responses were not corrected and all were mildly praised After seeing all six of the first set of faces, the experimenter introduced the second set.

Yes/No Task The two emotion categorization tasks, yes/no and choice-from array, were presented in counterbalanced order. On both categorizations tasks, the happy trial served as a control trial to ensure that children would indeed exclude anger and disgust faces from an emotion category.

The yes/no task was introduced as a game in which the experimenter was trying to be tricky and thus the child had to be particularly careful. In the training trial, the experimenter showed the child photographs of four different animals (dog, cat, horse, and cow), one at time in random Widen & Russell (ISRE 2006) 4 order. For each photograph, the experimenter asked, “Is this a dog?” The purpose of this training trial was encourage and reinforce the child for correctly saying ‘no’ to the experimenter.

Children’s performance on the animal trial was perfect for all four animals. Thus, they understood the task, and were willing and able to say ‘no’ to experimenter when it was appropriate to do so.

The next three trials followed the same format as the training trial, but now with four different facial expressions. On each trial, the four expressions were posed by the same person, and the experimenter asked the same question for all four facial expressions: “Is this person X?” (disgusted, angry, happy) The order of the trials was random, as was order of presentation of the three sets of photographs, and the order of the four photographs within each set.

Choice-from-array task In the choice-from-array task, six facial expressions (one each for happiness, sadness, anger, fear, surprise, disgust) for a given trial were presented at once, and the child was encouraged to look closely at each one. The experimenter then asked the child, “Which one of these people feels X?” (angry, disgusted, happy). The child’s selection was mildly praised and the experimenter removed that photograph, and then asked, “Does anyone else feel X? Or did you get them all?” This procedure was repeated until the child indicated that no one else felt the target emotion (or until there were no photographs left), and then the next trial was introduced with a new set of photographs.

Results To investigate whether emotion labels emerged systematically, all children were sorted and grouped, irrespective of age, by the emotion labels they used on the free labeling task. The frequency with which each target combination of emotion labels occurred was counted, and is illustrated by Figure 3. The frequency for each of the other combinations was low. This model accounted for 86.1% of the children – a proportion significantly greater (p.001) than the 27.1% expected by chance. (If, as the number of labels that children used increased, any label was as likely to be added as any other, then 27.1% would fit this pattern.) Results supported the Differentiation Model, except that, due perhaps slightly older age of the younger preschoolers in our sample, and the opportunity to label two sets of facial expressions, there were no children at Labeling Levels 0 or 1.

In the following analyses, children’s Labeling Level on free labeling was used to predict their performance on the yes/no and choice-from-array tasks. Labeling Levels 2, 3, and 4 were grouped together, as these Levels represent earlier stages of emotion understanding, Labeling Level 5 was considered a transitional level, and Labeling Level 6 was, of course, the focus Level at which children used disgusted, resulting in three groups: Labeling Level 2-4, Labeling Level 5, and Labeling Level 6. The adults were a fourth group. There were 22 participants in each group.

Categorization Tasks Yes/No task. In a weighted means, repeated measures ANOVA (alpha =.05), children’s Labeling Level (4 levels: Labeling Level 2-4, 5, 6, adults) was the between-subjects factor, and emotion (2 levels: anger, disgust), nested in trial (3 levels: angry, disgusted, happy), was the within subjectWiden & Russell (ISRE 2006) 5 factor for the yes/no task. The dependent variable was whether, or not, participants said ‘yes’ in response to the question, “Does this person feel X?” (scored 1 or 0, respectively). The trial x emotion x Labeling Level interaction was significant, F(6, 168) = 5.63, p.001. The happy



Pages:   || 2 |


Similar works:

«REX/219 Gemischter Beratender Ausschuss EU/Türkei Brüssel, den 6. Oktober 2006 21. Sitzung des Gemischten Beratenden Ausschusses EU/Türkei 13./14. Juli 2006 Kayseri, Türkei GEMEINSAMER BERICHT zum Thema FRAUEN UND BESCHÄFTIGUNG BERICHTERSTATTER Herr Jan OLSSON EWSA-Mitglied Gruppe Verschiedene Interessen Ko-Vorsitzender des Gemischten Beratenden Ausschusses EU/Türkei Senior Advisor, schwedisches Genossenschaftsinstitut (KOOPi) Vizepräsident des schwedischen Verbunds genossenschaftlicher...»

«Click here to view the fund's statutory prospectus or statement of additional information. Vanguard Extended Market ETF Summary Prospectus April 27 2016, Exchange-traded fund shares that are not individually redeemable and are listed on NYSE Arca Vanguard Extended Market Index Fund ETF Shares (VXF) The Fund’s statutory Prospectus and Statement of Additional Information dated April 27, 2016, as may be amended or supplemented, are incorporated into and made part of this Summary Prospectus by...»

«Beating the Misconceptions, Not the Children A Survey of Corporal Punishment in The Gambia Conducted by The Child Protection Alliance – The Gambia Official Representatives of ECPAT in The Gambia Funded by Save the Children – Sweden Regional Office West Africa Produced by: The Child Protection Alliance No. 8 Nora Senior Street Atlantic South Fajara, Bakau, The Gambia Tel: 220 4497425 E-mail: cpagambia@yahoo.com A Research Commissioned By Save the Children – Sweden West Africa Regional...»

«Sportverein Halle e.V. — Wasserball Robert Brode Tel.: 0049 345 1224163 mobil: 0049 1577 6079491 robertbrode@wasserball-halle.de Einladung zum Internationalen Wasserball – Turnier für Frauen, Jugendliche U15 (Jg.2001 und jünger) und Männer um den Herbstpokal 28. bis 30. Oktober 2016 Der SV Halle/ Abt. Wasserball veranstaltet vom 28.10. bis 30.10.2016 ein Wasserballturnier für Jugendliche und Erwachsene in der Schwimmhalle Halle-Neustadt. Trainingsspiele sind bereits am 27.10.2016...»

«Chapter 8: Dissertation Research and Writing – and the Defense A. Overview After you successfully defend your preliminary oral examination, you are considered a doctoral candidate (including a “pass with reservations”). Among the many activities of this phase of your graduate career are developing a dissertation time schedule and budget, securing funding for all phases of the dissertation project, conducting your research, analyzing and writing up your research, defending your...»

«Crime and Community Safety Reduce the risk of crime per 1,000 population There was an increase of 6.5% in total crime for the first three months of the Policing Plan Year, equating to an increase of 1324 crimes. Violence against the person increased by 1545 crimes, a rise of 37.7% compared to same period last year. Other increases in crime include sexual offences which increased by 96 (+24%) and burglary dwelling which increased by 32 crimes (4.5%). Burglary other than dwelling decreased by 65...»

«Fundamental J. Modern Physics, Vol. 1, Issue 2, 2011, Pages 169-195 Published online at http://www.frdint.com/ FRACTAL PHYSICS THEORY COSMIC SCALE NUCLEAR EXPLOSION COSMOLOGY LEONARD J. MALINOWSKI BASF Dispersions and Resins Monaca, Pennsylvania, USA Abstract This second article, in a series of five, begins to discuss Big Bang Cosmology in terms of a cosmic scale nuclear explosion in a decelerated time frame relative to the human scale. The very successful Big Bang model of the universe has...»

«American Antiquarian Society Paul Revere Collection Inventory Paul Revere Collection Inventory Boxes Revere Collection Box 1 Box 1 Folder 1 [Anatomical Lectures Certificate]. [mss. 1785]; sheet: (23.5 x 19 cm). plate: (19 x 15 cm). Link to record. Brigham plate 48. Link to Brigham.  Certificate  Chippendale style  Portrait  Scientific/astrological illustration Engraved certificate featuring a bust portrait of a bald Galen (AD 129 – 199/217) facing right surrounded by a...»

«Artistic Composition for Image Creation Bruce Gooch Erik Reinhard Chris Moulding Peter Shirley University of Utah Abstract. Altering the viewing parameters of a 3D object results in computer graphics images of varying quality. One aspect of image quality is the composition of the image. While the esthetic properties of an image are subjective, some heuristics used by artists to create images can be approximated quantitatively. We present an algorithm based on heuristic compositional rules for...»

«Print page 1. TABLE OF CONTENTS Abduction By Relative See Kidnapping Abuse, Animal Abuse, Child Abuse, Vulnerable Adult Accessory After The Fact Adultery Aircraft Explosive Alcohol AWOL Machine Retail Establishments Tax Intoxicated – Endanger Disturbance Drinking – Prohibited Places Minors – Citation Only Event – No Criminal Charges Open Container Proof of ID Ammo, Illegal Animal, Cruelty Race Horse. Animal, Law Enforcement Animal, Sale Appropriate Property Arson. Malicious Burning...»





 
<<  HOME   |    CONTACTS
2016 www.book.dislib.info - Free e-library - Books, dissertations, abstract

Materials of this site are available for review, all rights belong to their respective owners.
If you do not agree with the fact that your material is placed on this site, please, email us, we will within 1-2 business days delete him.